Behaviour change is often criticised for being too
mechanistic and harsh; more interested in the results and not the process. This
is, in part, a holdover from the halcyon days of lab-rat testing and
pidgeon-boxes of yore. Yet it is also down to the seemingly heartless way
businesses re-organise to maximise efficiency – oh and get rid of some ballast
along the way – and the way governments blithely ban or restrict activities
they see as inappropriate (or worse, politically useful) and so behaviour change
has become synonymous for some with things like the horrendous Soviet social
experiments, and the austere white-coats of scientists in lab prodding and
poking and shocking and starving animals all in the name of science.
But this is a myth.
Yes, once upon a time in days gone by behaviour was seen as
something mechanical, to be reduced to equations and symbols (you can thank
Watson and the Logical Positivists for that) but nowadays behaviour change is
anything but mechanistic and reductionist!
A brilliant paper by Delprato and Midgely explores
some of the aspects of Behaviour Analysis and put those (and other) myths to
rest. But nonetheless the myths persist.
We are seen as manipulative and controlling; for example
Nudge by Sunstein and Thaler was hailed as revolutionary in it’s simple
portrayal of complex ideas that appealed to a broad audience, it showed how we
can nudge behaviour to be better and
more effective without restricting the freedom of the individual.
Despite this the
reaction from some was vehement – to say the least. People were shocked that
governments could be inspired by such a supposedly totalitarian idea. This article here explains in the typically hyperbolic terms of sensationalist media how inevitably
the nudge will become a shove, citing Mayor Bloomberg of New York’s efforts to
control NY citizens behaviour, presumably alluding to the soda ban that hit the
headlines recently, but completely ignoring the fact that outright bans are
simply NOT allowed in Nudge theory.
So what can be done? Well better effort on our part to
down-play the talk of controlling and manipulating and instead highlight the
inherent freedom a behaviour change allows. The assumption of a lot of critics
is this; we are behaving rationally but not in the way we are “supposed to” as
defined by some shadowy agency. So along comes behaviour change techniques to
force us round pegs into some government-approved square holes. This is simply
not true; yes we can act rationally but we don’t. It’s learned. And it’s damn
hard to actually learn.
Second we have to re-assess our own goals within the
science. I am relatively new to academia and far be it from me to start
demanding sweeping changes in the way we think and act. Yet, it seems to me,
with the fresh eyes I bring, that a bit of positive PR would do us no end of
good. Not to mention a focus on inter-disciplinary efforts to make our work more
aesthetically pleasing to the man on the street, and in turn, more acceptable.
I don’t want to rattle on about this because it can become
dry quickly but it’s worth remembering that what is perceived as behaviour
change by the man on the Clapham omnibus and the scientists working on it can
sometimes be at odds. This is our fault for not portraying it in proper terms.
We aren’t against freedom, we aren’t against rationality, we aren’t against a
good life. We don’t want big brother, we don’t want intrusive regulations where
unnecessary, and we don’t want to control people like puppets.
I don’t have the answers, but maybe if we start asking the
right questions the answers will come.
Checkland's "soft systems" I remember being introduced to in the late 1980's as a riposte to the mechanistic approaches of the 60's and 70's.
ReplyDeleteI think he worked at Lancaster University if memory serves me correctly...